Jump to a key chapter
- We will delve into the topic of the different levels of moral reasoning and cognitive distortions.
- First, we will define cognitive distortions, providing examples of cognitive distortions and how they relate to forensic psychology.
- Then, we will discuss levels of moral reasoning, focusing specifically on Kohlberg.
- Finally, we will highlight the strengths and weaknesses of each topic in an evaluation.
Types of Cognitive Distortions
Cognitive distortions are abnormal behaviours or thoughts that alter a person's perception of reality. Naturally, this can directly affect behaviour and how people interpret situations. Cognitive distortions are irrational thoughts that cause you to perceive reality negatively, even though reality does not reflect this.
John Gibbs et al. (1995) established four types of cognitive distortions common in criminals:
- Self-centredness.
- Blaming others.
- Minimising issues.
- Hostile attribution bias (assuming the worst).
Cognitive distortions go hand in hand with moral reasoning, especially concerning criminal behaviour, as they cause potentially abnormal thought processes and behaviours.Jean Piaget studied the cognitive abilities children develop to make moral judgments. Later, Kohlberg and James Rest continued to study moral reasoning abilities in adults. Kohlberg was the first researcher to apply moral judgement to criminal behaviour.
Moral reasoning is the cognitive process of reasoning whilst considering ethical implications, also known as right and wrong. The perspective recognises right and wrong and acknowledges the rules of social conduct.
Examples of Cognitive Distortions
Cognitive distortions may explain the moral reasoning behind some offences. Gibbs et al. (1995) identified cognitive distortions of particular importance in offending behaviours. These irrational distortions are problematic because they affect thoughts and behaviour because the distorted view of reality leads to abnormal behaviour.
Let's explore the different types of cognitive distortions further.
Hostile Attribution Bias
In the context of criminal behaviour, the hostile attribution of bias is related to the offender’s misinterpretation of other people’s attitudes by typically assuming the worst. It is assumed that others will behave aggressively and confrontationally toward the perpetrator when this is not the case. This assumption often leads to disproportionate and aggressive responses.
Michael Schonenberg and Aiste Jusyte (2014) conducted a study in which they presented 55 violent offenders with pictures of emotionally ambiguous facial expressions and compared them to a nonaggressive control group.
They found that violent offenders were more likely to recognise anger and hostility in the images than non-violent offenders.
Kenneth Dodge and Cynthia Frame (1982) believed that the origin of these cognitive biases might stem from childhood. They conducted a study showing children a video clip of ambiguous, provocative situations in which the behaviour was neither hostile nor random. Before the study, the children who were classified as aggressive perceived the video clip as more aggressive overall than those who were classified as nonaggressive.
Minimalisation
Research shows that sex offenders try to minimise their guilt and sometimes even shift the blame to the victims.
Minimalisation is a type of cognitive distortion. The person tries to minimise or reduce the severity of their crimes and behaviours. In other words, it can be seen as denial or self-deception. Minimising the seriousness of the situation emphasises a refusal to accept what one has done. The consequences are exaggerated (either under or over-exaggerated) to some degree.
An example of minimalisation is if someone were to steal from a shop and then describe their criminal act as a job. They would justify their misconduct by saying that it is their family’s source of income, and they had to do it to provide for their family. Doing so is a way of downplaying the seriousness of the crime.
Kennedy and Grubin (1992) found most convicted sex offenders tend to blame the victim for the crime. Another quarter believed that it had ended positively for the victim. Some thought no harm was done to the victim (especially the case of offenders who had sexually assaulted children).
Howard Barbaree (1991) found that of 26 incarcerated rapists, 54% completely denied the crime. The other 40% justified the harm they inflicted on victims by downplaying it.
Kohlberg’s Stages of Moral Development: Ages
Kohlberg agreed with Piaget’s initial interpretation of children’s moral development and wanted to expand on these points. This led to Kohlberg’s stages of moral development, detailing three levels of reasoning comprised of six overall stages. Kohlberg's stages of moral development occur around the following ages:
- Level One (Pre-Conventional Reasoning): Obedience and punishment AND Self-interest - Nine years old.
- Level Two (Conventional Reasoning): Good people orientation AND Law and order - Older children, adolescents, and most adults.
- Level Three (Postconventional Reasoning): Social contract orientation AND Universal ethics principle - Rarely adolescents, some adults.
Most people tend to end up in level two, and rarely do people reach level three. Kohlberg developed and investigated his stages of moral development theory through ten moral stories presented to participants. One famous example is the Heinz Dilemma.
The Heinz Dilemma details a moral dilemma where participants had to decide if Heinz's actions were justified based on the story.
Heinz had a sick wife and needed expensive medication to save her. Unfortunately, he could not afford it, so broke into the shop with the expensive medication to steal it to help save his dying wife.
Kohlberg asked participants if the actions were justified and the reasoning behind the participant's decisions.
Level One – Preconventional Reasoning
Preconventional reasoning is the first stage of moral development in which the sense of moral values is externally directed.
- Children are in this stage until they are about nine years old.
Children do not yet have a personal moral code at this stage because they have not internalised social conventions about right and wrong. Instead, adult norms and the consequences of breaking their rules guide moral decisions.
Stage One: Obedience and Punishment
Children understand rules based on how authority figures enact rewards or punishments. Actions associated with ‘offending behaviour’ or breaking rules at this stage are:
If my actions result in punishment, it must have been a wrong decision.
If my actions lead to a reward, it must be the right decision.
Stage Two: Self-Interest
Children’s best interest determines the proper behaviour at this stage, so they show little interest in others and are concerned with their gain. Actions associated with ‘offending behaviour’ or breaking rules at this stage are:
What are the gains of breaking the rules?
The offence is likely to occur if the potential gain is worth it.
Level Two – Conventional Reasoning
Conventional reasoning is the second stage of moral development. The sense of morality at the conventional level is tied to personal and social relationships.
Children and adolescents accept the rules of authority figures because they provide positive relationships and maintain social order.
It is the stage when people begin to internalise the moral standards of adult roles in society. Attachment to rules and conventions is more rigid, and the rule’s relevance or integrity is rarely questioned. At this stage, social rules are essential to ensure the functioning of society. However, this affects our moral views of what is right and wrong.
Stage Three: Good People Orientation
In this stage, the emphasis is on positive behaviour and a good and balanced relationship with others. The individual tends to be good so that others perceive them as such.
Therefore, this positive behaviour is strongly associated with others’ recognition. Children need the approval of peers and authority figures and will behave accordingly to avoid being disliked.
Stage Four: Law and Order Orientation
At this stage, the child accepts rules and conventions due to the importance of maintaining a functioning society. The laws are seen as equal to everyone, and obeying rules is considered valuable and essential.
Moral reasoning is about the need for individual approval, like in the previous stage. There is a duty to support laws and rules.
Most people in society remain at stage four, where an outside force dictates morality.
Level Three: Postconventional Reasoning
Postconventional morality is the third level of moral development. At this level, people develop their own ethical and moral principles. At this stage, the individual understands some laws are unjust and should be changed or abolished, characterised by a growing awareness.
These may be abstract ethical principles and values, usually aimed at the greater good of humanity.
Postconventional moralists have their ethical principles. These principles usually rely on ensuring the welfare of the most significant number of people possible and prioritising human rights. Postconventionalists place their moral evaluation of a situation above social conventions. Some theorists assume that many people will not reach such a level of abstract moral reasoning.
Stage Five: Social Contract Orientation
Kohlberg believed this stage was unattainable for most, where the world is diverse with different cultures, values, rights, and opinions. Perspectives at this level should be mutual and respected as unique to each.
Individuals see laws not as rigid edicts but as social agreements. People at this stage should allow and encourage changes in cooperation for the highest good of the general welfare.
The five reasons are the theoretical principles of democratic government.
Stage Six: Universal Ethical Principles
Moral reasoning relies on the abstract application of universal ethical principles at this stage. Usually, these principles focus on moral values such as equality, dignity, or respect.
At this stage, people believe laws are valid only if they rely on justice. They are aware that commitment to justice entails not obeying unjust laws. People who choose ethical principles want to follow those laws.
When they violate them, they feel remorse. They select certain attitudes because they believe it is in their best interest. Kohlberg believed that at this stage, it is difficult to identify individuals who consistently act at this level.
Ma (2013) found the first four stages nearly the same across populations, but stages five and six were scarce.
Importance of Kohlberg's Theory of Moral Development
Kohlberg was the first researcher to apply moral reasoning to criminal behaviour and proposed three levels of moral reasoning. The higher the level, the more complex the stages of moral development become.
Difference Between Cognitive Development and Moral Reasoning
Moral reasoning development is a product of cognitive development. We can see how cognitive development and moral reasoning tie in together by understanding how criminals develop different moral outlooks.
It is theorised that criminals do not progress through the stages of moral reasoning and cognitive development milestones like other members of society. Instead, they remain at lower levels of moral reasoning (Allen et al., 2001).
The preconventional stage links to the need to avoid punishment and receive rewards. It is also associated with less mature reasonings. People classified at this level are more likely to commit a crime if they can avoid responsibility or punishment due to their actions or if they can be rewarded due to their misbehaviour.
Evaluation of Moral Reasoning and Cognitive Distortions
Understanding the strengths and weaknesses of the approaches discussed above is essential.
Strengths of Levels of Moral Reasoning Approach
Research supports the cognitive explanation for offending behaviour. Palmer and Hollin (1998) compared the moral reasoning of female and male nonoffenders and offenders.
They found the offender group exhibited lower moral reasoning maturity than the nonoffender group, which was the same for both genders. Female nonoffenders showed higher moral judgement than male nonoffenders.
Strengths of the Cognitive Distortions Approach
Crick and Dodge (1994) found a relationship between hostile attribution bias and aggression observed in children and adolescents. The link between actual situations is considered one of the antecedents of aggressive behaviour in children, adolescents, and adults, possibly leading to criminal behaviour.
Another strength of the cognitive explanation of delinquent behaviour is that it is widely applicable.
For example, understanding cognitive distortions have been shown to be beneficial in treating criminal behaviour, particularly in rehabilitating sex offenders for whom cognitive behavioural therapy is used.
The rehabilitation included cognitive behavioural therapy methods that directly address dysfunctional thoughts (cognitive distortions). They learn how to gain a less distorted view of their actions by confronting their crime. Studies found less denial and minimisation in therapy are highly correlated with a lower risk of recidivism, i.e., reoffending.
Weaknesses of the Cognitive Distortions Approach
Minimalisation can be seen as descriptive rather than explanatory. It describes how an offender distorts the reality of their offending behaviour but does not explain the reasons for committing the crime. However, minimalisation could predict reoffending.
Weaknesses of Levels of Moral Reasoning Approach
The cognitive explanation for delinquent behaviour using the moral reasoning model is gender-specific because it was conducted on American males.
- Gilligan (1982) thought the model was too androcentric.
- Gibbs (1979) suggested Kohlberg’s postconventional level should be abandoned because it is culturally biased toward Western culture.
This is problematic because the model does not represent a ‘natural’ maturation stage of cognitive development. Alternatively, Gibbs proposed two levels of moral reasoning: the mature and the immature. These levels tend to be more general when applied to different cultures. It questions the credibility of the cognitive explanation of offending behaviour.
Level of Moral Reasoning and Cognitive Distortions - Key takeaways
- Cognitive moral development, also known as moral reasoning, comes from cognitive development and levels of morality in psychology.
- Kohlberg’s stages of moral development detail three levels of moral reasoning comprised of six overall stages. Kohlberg was the first researcher to apply moral reasoning to criminal behaviour, suggesting criminals have a lower level of moral reasoning.
- Kohlberg's three levels of moral reasoning are preconventional reasoning (level one), conventional reasoning (level two), and postconventional reasoning (level three). Most people tend to reach level two and rarely do people reach level three.
- Cognitive distortions are abnormal behaviours or thoughts that alter a person's perception of reality. Cognitive distortions can potentially explain criminal behaviour. They include minimisation, being self-centred, hostile attribution bias, and blaming others.
- Research supporting Kohlberg’s theory suggests offenders are often more selfish and have a poorer ability to adopt social perspective-taking skills than nonoffenders. Research supporting cognitive distortions demonstrates the link between distortions and delinquent behaviour. Both theories have weaknesses, too.
References
- Fig. 2 - Stages of Moral Reasoning Table by Lawrence Kohlberg, Em Griffin, cmglee, CC BY-SA 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/4.0, via Wikimedia Commons
Learn faster with the 0 flashcards about Level of Moral Reasoning and Cognitive Distortions
Sign up for free to gain access to all our flashcards.
Frequently Asked Questions about Level of Moral Reasoning and Cognitive Distortions
What are the levels of moral reasoning and cognitive distortions?
According to Kohlberg, the levels of moral reasoning are:
- Level One (Pre-Conventional Reasoning): Obedience and punishment AND Self-interest - Nine years old.
- Level Two (Conventional Reasoning): Good people orientation AND Law and order - Older children, adolescents, and most adults.
- Level Three (Postconventional Reasoning): Social contract orientation AND Universal ethics principle - Rarely adolescents, some adults.
What are neural explanations of offending behaviour?
Neural explanations suggest someone’s moral reasoning for crime is linked to neurological factors.
What are the examples of the level of moral reasoning and cognitive distortions?
An example of someone with cognitive distortions is when they assume that someone looking at them is doing so to pick a fight; they were aggressive and did not just happen to make eye contact. Similarly, a person can construe a smile as aggressive.
An example of a level in moral reasoning is preconventional reasoning level (level one). Children understand rules based on how authority figures enact rewards or punishments in this stage.
What is stage five of Kohlberg's theory?
Stage Five: Social Contract Orientation
Kohlberg believed this stage was unattainable for most, where the world is diverse with different cultures, values, rights, and opinions. Perspectives at this level should be mutual and respected as unique to each.
Individuals see laws not as rigid edicts but as social agreements. People at this stage should allow and encourage changes in cooperation for the highest good of the general welfare.
What is the difference between cognitive development and moral reasoning?
Moral reasoning development is a product of cognitive development.
About StudySmarter
StudySmarter is a globally recognized educational technology company, offering a holistic learning platform designed for students of all ages and educational levels. Our platform provides learning support for a wide range of subjects, including STEM, Social Sciences, and Languages and also helps students to successfully master various tests and exams worldwide, such as GCSE, A Level, SAT, ACT, Abitur, and more. We offer an extensive library of learning materials, including interactive flashcards, comprehensive textbook solutions, and detailed explanations. The cutting-edge technology and tools we provide help students create their own learning materials. StudySmarter’s content is not only expert-verified but also regularly updated to ensure accuracy and relevance.
Learn more